دورية أكاديمية

Irrelevant-action imitation is short-term and contextual: Evidence from two under-studied populations.

التفاصيل البيبلوغرافية
العنوان: Irrelevant-action imitation is short-term and contextual: Evidence from two under-studied populations.
المؤلفون: Kline MA; Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada., Gervais MM; Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada.; Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Burnaby, BC, Canada., Moya C; Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA., Boyd RT; Institute of Human Origins, School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA.
المصدر: Developmental science [Dev Sci] 2020 May; Vol. 23 (3), pp. e12903. Date of Electronic Publication: 2019 Oct 01.
نوع المنشور: Journal Article; Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural; Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S.
اللغة: English
بيانات الدورية: Publisher: Wiley-Blackwell Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 9814574 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1467-7687 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 1363755X NLM ISO Abbreviation: Dev Sci Subsets: MEDLINE
أسماء مطبوعة: Original Publication: Oxford, UK ; Malden, MA, USA : Wiley-Blackwell, c1998-
مواضيع طبية MeSH: Imitative Behavior* , Models, Theoretical* , Social Environment*, Adult ; Age Factors ; Child ; Child Behavior ; Culture ; Female ; Fiji ; Humans ; Learning ; Male ; Peru
مستخلص: Across the lifespan and across populations, humans 'overimitate' causally unnecessary behaviors. Such irrelevant-action imitation facilitates faithful cultural transmission, but its immediate benefits to the imitator are controversial. Over short time scales, irrelevant-action imitation may bootstrap artifact exploration or interpersonal affiliation, and over longer time scales it may facilitate acquisition of either causal models or social conventions. To investigate these putative functions, we recruited community samples from two under-studied populations: Yasawa, Fiji, and Huatasani, Peru. We use a two-action puzzle box: first after a video demonstration, and again one month later. Treating age as a continuous variable, we reveal divergent developmental trajectories across sites. Yasawans (44 adults, M = 39.9 years, 23 women; 42 children, M = 9.8 years, 26 girls) resemble documented patterns, with irrelevant-action imitation increasing across childhood and plateauing in adulthood. In contrast, Huatasaneños (48 adults, M = 37.6 years, 33 women; 47 children, M = 9.3 years, 13 girls) evince a parabolic trajectory: adults at the site show the lowest irrelevant-action imitation of any demographic set in our sample. In addition, all age sets in both populations reduce their irrelevant actions at Time 2, but do not reduce their relevant-action imitation or goal attainment. Taken together, and considering the local cultural contexts, our results suggest that irrelevant-action imitation serves a short-term function and is sensitive to the social context of the demonstration.
(© 2019 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.)
References: Aunger, R. (2000). The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos, 28(3), 445-481. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.2000.28.3.445.
Barton, K. (2017). MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R Package Version 1.5.
Berl, R. E. W., & Hewlett, B. S. (2015). Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0120180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120180.s007.
Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401-432). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bissell, W. C. (2005). Engaging colonial nostalgia. Cultural Anthropology, 20(2), 215-248. https://doi.org/10.1525/can.2005.20.2.215.
Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322-330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001.
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). Colloquium paper: The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(Supplement_2), 10918-10925. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108.
Boyette, A. H. (2013). Social learning during middle childhood among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Central African Republic, Unpublished Dissertation, Washington State University, Vancouver, WA.
Brison, K. T. (2003). Imagining modernity in rural FIji. Ethnology, 42(4), 335-348. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773833.
Buchsbaum, D., Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Shafto, P. (2011). Children's imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence. Cognition, 120(3), 331-340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.001.
Bullock, M., & Gelman, R. (1979). Preschool children's assumptions about cause and effect: Temporal ordering. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50(1), 89-96. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129045.
Burdett, E. R. R., McGuigan, N., Harrison, R., & Whiten, A. (2018). The interaction of social and perceivable causal factors in shaping “over-imitation”. Cognitive Development, 47, 8-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.02.001.
Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. (2003). Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretic approach (2nd edn). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Chudek, M., Baron, A. S., & Birch, S. (2016). Unselective overimitators: The evolutionary implications of children's indiscriminate copying of successful and prestigious models. Child Development, 87(3), 782-794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12529.
Clay, Z., Over, H., & Tennie, C. (2018). What drives young children to over-imitate? Investigating the effects of age, context, action type, and transitivity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 520-534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.008.
Clay, Z., & Tennie, C. (2017). Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Development, 89(5), 1535-1544. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12857.
Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016). A cross-cultural comparison of children’s imitative flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1435-1444. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000131.
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 1114-1118. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1213969.
Dell, M. (2010). The persistent effects of Peru's mining mita. Econometrica, 78(6), 1863-1903. https://doi.org/10.3982/ECTA8121.
Demps, K., Zorondo-Rodríguez, F., García, C., & Reyes-Garcia, V. (2012). Social learning across the life cycle: Cultural knowledge acquisition for honey collection among the Jenu Kuruba, India. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 460-470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.008.
Evans, C. L., Laland, K. N., Carpenter, M., & Kendal, R. L. (2017). Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly “optimal-” rather than ‘over-’ imitators. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12637-e12710. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12637.
Flynn, E., Turner, C., & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2016). Selectivity in social and asocial learning: Investigating the prevalence, effect and development of young children's learning preferences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150189-20150198. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0189.
Gardiner, A. K. (2014). Beyond irrelevant actions: understanding the role of intentionality in children's imitation of relevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 54-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.008.
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755. https://doi.org/10.1038/415755a.
Harris, P. L., & Corriveau, K. H. (2011). Young children's selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1179-1187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01356.x.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 123-135. https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1520-6505.
Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). “Over-imitation”_ A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002.
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164-181. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1213969.
Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing. Social Cognition, 27, 949-961.
Kent, R. B. (1993). Geographical dimensions of the shining path insurgency in Peru. Geographical Review, 441-454. https://doi.org/10.2307/215825.
Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Current Opinion in Psychology, 112(2), 195-207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006.
Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2010). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1709), 1239-1246. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1399.
Keupp, S., Bancken, C., Schillmöller, J., Rakoczy, H., & Behne, T. (2016). Rational over-imitation: Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively. Cognition, 147, 85-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.11.007.
Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 392-406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002.
Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2010). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14(1), 106-111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences USA, 104(30), 12577-12580. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705345104.
Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24(4), 351-374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9180-1.
Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: Making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743), 20170059-20170068. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0059.
Lal, B. (1992). Broken waves: A history of the Fiji Islands in the twentieth century, Vol. 11. Honolulu: University of Hawai'i Press.
Laland, K. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning and Behavior, 32(1), 4-14. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196002.
Lancy, D. F., & Grove, M. A. (2011). Getting noticed. Human Nature, 22(3), 281-302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-011-9117-5.
Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142(C), 351-361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.020.
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(50), 19751-19756. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704452104.
Manson, J. H., Bryant, G. A., Gervais, M. M., & Kline, M. A. (2013). Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 419-426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.001.
McElreath, R. (2018). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. New York: CRC Press.
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122-129. https://doi.org/10.1086/345689.
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X493115.
McGuigan, N., & Robertson, S. (2015). The influence of peers on the tendency of 3-and 4-year-old children to over-imitate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 42-54.
McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & Horner, V. (2007). Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22(3), 353-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.01.001.
Mills, C. M. (2013). Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 404-418. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029500.
Moraru, C.-A., Gomez, J.-C., & McGuigan, N. (2016). Developmental changes in the influence of conventional and instrumental cues on over-imitation in 3- to 6-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145, 34-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.017.
Moya, C. (2013). Evolved priors for ethnolinguistic categorization: A case study from the Quechua-Aymara boundary in the Peruvian Altiplano. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 265-272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.03.004.
Moya, C., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2015). Reasoning about cultural and genetic transmission: Developmental and cross-cultural evidence from Peru, Fiji, and the United States on how people make inferences about trait transmission. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(4), 595-610. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12163.
Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kartner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 31-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017.
Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014a). Where culture takes hold: “Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in western, aboriginal, and bushmen children. Child Development. 85(6):2169-2184. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12265.
Nielsen, M., & Susianto, E. W. (2010). Failure to find over-imitation in captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus): Implications for our understanding of cross-generation information transfer. In J. Hakansson (Ed.), Developmental Psychology (pp. 153-167). New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21(5), 729-736. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610368808.
Nielsen, M., Tomaselli, K., Mushin, I., & Whiten, A. (2014b). Exploring tool innovation: A comparison of Western and Bushman children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 384-394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.05.008.
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012a). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182-192. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024555.
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012b). The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 6-11. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12006.
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(Supplement_2), 8993-8999. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914630107.
R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/.
Reed, A. R. (2016). Nostalgia in the post-apartheid state. Anthropology Southern Africa, 39(2), 97-109. https://doi.org/10.1080/02533952.2013.852371.
Rogoff, B., Sellers, M. J., Pirrotta, S., Fox, N., & White, S. H. (1975). Age of assignment of roles and responsibilities to children. Human Development, 18(5). https://doi.org/10.1159/000271496.
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: Corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1234-1246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1234.
Shneidman, L., Gweon, H., Schulz, L. E., & Woodward, A. L. (2016). Learning from others and spontaneous exploration: A cross-cultural investigation. Child Development, 87(3), 723-735. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12502.
Simpson, A., & Riggs, K. J. (2011). Three- and 4-year-olds encode modeled actions in two ways leading to immediate imitation and delayed emulation. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 834-840. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023270.
Stef van Buuren, K.-G.-O. (2011). Journal of Statistical Software, 1-67.
Stroebe, W., & Strack, F. (2014). The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 59-71. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460687.
Taniguchi, Y., & Sanefuji, W. (2017). The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children: Effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 83-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.014.
Veramu, J. (1986). Fijian education, the community and the teacher. Directions. Journal of Educational Studies, 16.
Vivanti, G., Hocking, D. R., Fanning, P., & Dissanayake, C. (2017). The social nature of overimitation: Insights from Autism and Williams syndrome. Cognition, 161, 10-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.008.
Walker, C. F. (2014). The Tupac Amaru Rebellion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Watson-Jones, R. E., Legare, C. H., Whitehouse, H., & Clegg, J. M. (2014). Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(3), 204-210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.01.004.
Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27(1), 34-42. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615607205.
Weismantel, M. (2001). Cholas and pishtacos: Stories of race and sex in the Andes. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Whiten, A., Allan, G., Devlin, S., Kseib, N., Raw, N., & McGuigan, N. (2016). Social Learning in the Real-World: “Over-Imitation” Occurs in Both Children and Adults Unaware of Participation in an Experiment and Independently of Social Interaction. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0159920. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159920.s001.
Whiten, A., & Erdal, D. (2012). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2119-2129. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0114.
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2417-2428. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069.
Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2013a). Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning. Cognition, 127(2), 203-213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.002.
Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2013b). Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning. Developmental Review, 33(4), 341-356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.002.
Yu, Y., & Kushnir, T. (2014). Social context effects in 2- and 4-year-olds’ selective versus faithful imitation. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 922-933. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034242.
معلومات مُعتمدة: International National Institute of Health; International National Science Foundation; International Leakey Foundation; International John Templeton Foundation
فهرسة مساهمة: Keywords: culture and development; overimitation; social learning
تواريخ الأحداث: Date Created: 20190911 Date Completed: 20201105 Latest Revision: 20201105
رمز التحديث: 20221213
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12903
PMID: 31505090
قاعدة البيانات: MEDLINE
الوصف
تدمد:1467-7687
DOI:10.1111/desc.12903