دورية أكاديمية

"Speak of the Devil… and he Shall Appear": Religiosity, Unconsciousness, and the Effects of Explicit Priming in the Misperception of Immorality.

التفاصيل البيبلوغرافية
العنوان: "Speak of the Devil… and he Shall Appear": Religiosity, Unconsciousness, and the Effects of Explicit Priming in the Misperception of Immorality.
المؤلفون: Tsikandilakis M; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK. tsikandilakismyron@gmail.com.; Medical School, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK. tsikandilakismyron@gmail.com., Leong MQ; Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore., Yu Z; Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore. zhaoliang@u.nus.edu., Paterakis G; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Bali P; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Derrfuss J; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Mevel PA; Department of Modern Languages and Cultures, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Milbank A; Department of Theology and Religious Studies, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Tong EMW; Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore., Madan C; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK., Mitchell P; School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK.
المصدر: Psychological research [Psychol Res] 2022 Feb; Vol. 86 (1), pp. 37-65. Date of Electronic Publication: 2021 Jan 23.
نوع المنشور: Journal Article
اللغة: English
بيانات الدورية: Publisher: Springer-Verlag Country of Publication: Germany NLM ID: 0435062 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1430-2772 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 03400727 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Psychol Res Subsets: MEDLINE
أسماء مطبوعة: Original Publication: Berlin, New York, Springer-Verlag.
مواضيع طبية MeSH: Morals* , Religion*, Bayes Theorem ; Cognition ; Humans ; Male ; Unconsciousness
مستخلص: Psychological theory and research suggest that religious individuals could have differences in the appraisal of immoral behaviours and cognitions compared to non-religious individuals. This effect could occur due to adherence to prescriptive and inviolate deontic religious-moral rules and socio-evolutionary factors, such as increased autonomic nervous system responsivity to indirect threat. The latter thesis has been used to suggest that immoral elicitors could be processed subliminally by religious individuals. In this manuscript, we employed masking to test this hypothesis. We rated and pre-selected IAPS images for moral impropriety. We presented these images masked with and without negatively manipulating a pre-image moral label. We measured detection, moral appraisal and discrimination, and physiological responses. We found that religious individuals experienced higher responsivity to masked immoral images. Bayesian and hit-versus-miss response analyses revealed that the differences in appraisal and physiological responses were reported only for consciously perceived immoral images. Our analysis showed that when a negative moral label was presented, religious individuals experienced the interval following the label as more physiologically arousing and responded with lower specificity for moral discrimination. We propose that religiosity involves higher conscious perceptual and physiological responsivity for discerning moral impropriety but also higher susceptibility for the misperception of immorality.
(© 2021. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE part of Springer Nature.)
References: Admin HR. (2019, October 12). Human resources. Retrieved from https://www.hr.admin.cam.ac.uk/pay-benefits/salary-scales .
Ahmed, A. M., & Salas, O. (2008). In the back of your mind: Subliminal influences of religious concepts on prosocial behavior. rapport nr.: Working Papers in Economics 331.
Alexithymia. (2018, February 12). Alexithymia: Emotional blindness. Retrieved from http://www.alexithymia.us/test-alex.html .
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: APA. (PMID: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596)
Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Axelrod, V., Bar, M., & Rees, G. (2015). Exploring the unconscious using faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(1), 35–45. (PMID: 25481216)
Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on psychological science, 3(1), 73–79.
Bachmann, T., & Francis, G. (2013). Visual masking: Studying perception, attention, and consciousness. Academic Press.
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 29–34. (PMID: 10637620)
Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013). Explaining moral religions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 272–280. (PMID: 23664451)
Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I., & Boyer, P. (2015). Increased affluence explains the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Current Biology, 25(1), 10–15. (PMID: 25496963)
Beal, T. K. (2002). Religion and its monsters. New York: Psychology Press.
Berry, D. A. (1996). Statistics: a Bayesian perspective. London: Duxbury Press.
Birgegard, A., & Granqvist, P. (2004). The correspondence between attachment to parents and God: Three experiments using subliminal separation cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1122–1135. (PMID: 15359016)
Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. B. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research PRPF, 73(6), 835–842.
Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on religion. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 111–130.
Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595–613.
Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., & Rowe, M. (2013). A guide for analyzing electrodermal activity (EDA) and skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Psychophysiology, 49(1), 1–43.
Brooks, S. J., Savov, V., Allzén, E., Benedict, C., Fredriksson, R., & Schiöth, H. B. (2012). Exposure to subliminal arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular cortex and primary visual cortex: a systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2962–2973. (PMID: 22001789)
Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Carlson, J. M., Fee, A. L., & Reinke, K. S. (2009). Backward masked snakes and guns modulate spatial attention. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 1414.
Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: A standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1178–1198. (PMID: 255828114780680)
Cohen, A. B., Malka, A., Rozin, P., & Cherfas, L. (2006). Religion and unforgivable offenses. Journal of Personality, 74(1), 85–118. (PMID: 16451227)
Cohen, A. B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 697. (PMID: 11642355)
Colden, A., Bruder, M., & Manstead, A. S. (2008). Human content in affect-inducing stimuli: A secondary analysis of the international affective picture system. Motivation and Emotion, 32(4), 260–269.
Crone, D. L., Bode, S., Murawski, C., & Laham, S. M. (2018). The Socio-Moral Image Database (SMID): A novel stimulus set for the study of social, moral and affective processes. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0190954. (PMID: 293649855783374)
Davies, C. L., Sibley, C. G., & Liu, J. H. (2014). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Moral Foundations Questionnaire. Social Psychology, 45, 431–435.
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. (PMID: 16603406)
Dehaene, S., Lau, H., & Kouider, S. (2017). What is consciousness, and could machines have it? Science, 358(6362), 486–492. (PMID: 29074769)
Dienes, Z. (2011). Bayesian versus Orthodox statistics: Which side are you on? Perspectives on Psychological Sciences, 6(3), 274–290.
Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 781. (PMID: 251205034114196)
Dienes, Z. (2016). How Bayes factors change scientific practice. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 72, 78–89.
Dienes, Z. (2019). How do I know what my theory predicts? Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 56–73.
Dijksterhuis, A., Preston, J., Wegner, D. M., & Aarts, H. (2008). Effects of subliminal priming of self and God on self-attribution of authorship for events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 2–9.
Durkheim, E. (1965). The elementary forms of the religious life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ebstyne King, P., & Furrow, J. L. (2008, August). Religion as a resource for positive youth development: Religion, social capital, and moral outcomes. In: Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Apr, 2001, Minneapolis, MN, USA; A previous version of this article was presented at the aforementioned conference. (No. 1, p. 34). Educational Publishing Foundation, Washington.
Ellemers, N., van der Toorn, J., Paunov, Y., & van Leeuwen, T. (2019). The psychology of morality: A review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personality and Social Psychology Review., 23, 332–366. (PMID: 306585456791030)
Erdelyi, M. H. (2004). Subliminal perception and its cognates: Theory, indeterminacy, and time. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(1), 73–91. (PMID: 14990242)
Everett, J. A. (2013). The 12 item social and economic conservatism scale (SECS). PLoS ONE, 8(12), e82131. (PMID: 243492003859575)
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. (PMID: 1989782319897823)
FederalPay (2019, October 12). Federal Pay Resources. Retrieved from https://www.federalpay.org/ .
Freedberg, D. (1989). The power of images: Studies in the history and theory of response. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Freud, S. (1950). Totem and taboo (1913). Standard Edition, 13(1), 161.
Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L., & Langdon, P. E. (2016). The neural correlates of moral decision-making: A systematic review and meta-analysis of moral evaluations and response decision judgements. Brain and Cognition, 108, 88–97. (PMID: 27566002)
Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2011). Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion, 41(3), 389–410.
Graham, J., & Haidt, J. (2010). Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 140–150. (PMID: 20089848)
Gray, K. L., Adams, W. J., Hedger, N., Newton, K. E., & Garner, M. (2013). Faces and awareness: Low-level, not emotional factors determine perceptual dominance. Emotion, 13(3), 537. (PMID: 23398580)
The Guardian. (2019, February 12). Why religion has become so popular in recent days and what happens next? Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/news/2018/aug/27/religion-why-is-faith-growing-and-what-happens-next .
Haidt, J. (2003). Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. Flourishing Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived, 275, 289.
Hajian-Tilaki, K. O., Hanley, J. A., Joseph, L., & Collet, J. P. (1997). A comparison of parametric and nonparametric approaches to ROC analysis of quantitative diagnostic tests. Medical Decision Making, 17(1), 94–102. (PMID: 8994156)
Harenski, C. L., Antonenko, O., Shane, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010). A functional imaging investigation of moral deliberation and moral intuition. Neuroimage, 49(3), 2707–2716. (PMID: 19878727)
Harenski, C. L., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage, 30(1), 313–324. (PMID: 16249098)
Hickie, I. B., Davenport, T. A., Hadzi-Paviovic, D., Koschera, A., Naismith, S. L., Scott, E. M., & Wilhelm, K. A. (2001). Development of a simple screening tool for common mental disorders in general practice. Medical Journal of Australia, 175, S10–S17.
Howard, S., & Sommers, S. R. (2017). Exposure to White religious iconography influences Black individuals’ intragroup and intergroup attitudes. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23(4), 508. (PMID: 28394162)
Huang, M. A., Sun, H. M., & Vaina, L. M. (2019). Visual attributes of subliminal priming images impact conscious perception of facial expressions. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 9, 108–120.
Jerram, M., Lee, A., Negreira, A., & Gansler, D. (2014). The neural correlates of the dominance dimension of emotion. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 221(2), 135–141. (PMID: 24359970)
Kim, M. J., Loucks, R. A., Neta, M., Davis, F. C., Oler, J. A., Mazzulla, E. C., & Whalen, P. J. (2010). Behind the mask: the influence of mask-type on amygdala response to fearful faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(4), 363–368. (PMID: 201474562999762)
Kimura, Y., Yoshino, A., Takahashi, Y., & Nomura, S. (2004). Interhemispheric difference in emotional response without awareness. Physiology and Behavior, 82(4), 727–731. (PMID: 15327923)
Koenig, H. G., Youssef, N. A., Ames, D., Oliver, J. P., Teng, E. J., Haynes, K., & Pearce, M. (2018). Moral injury and religiosity in US veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 206(5), 325–331. (PMID: 29494381)
Kret, M. E., & De Gelder, B. (2012). A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1211–1221. (PMID: 22245006)
Krupinski, E. A. (2017). Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) Analysis. Frontline Learning Research, 5(3), 41–52.
Lang, P., & Bradley, M. M. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, 29, 70–73.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1, 39–58.
Lewinski, P., den Uyl, T. M., & Butler, C. (2014). Automated facial coding: Validation of basic emotions and FACS AUs in FaceReader. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(4), 227.
Lewis-Evans, B., De Waard, D., Jolij, J., & Brookhuis, K. A. (2012). What you may not see might slow you down anyway: Masked images and driving. PLoS ONE, 7(1), e29857. (PMID: 222795493261158)
Luo, Q., Nakic, M., Wheatley, T., Richell, R., Martin, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2006). The neural basis of implicit moral attitude—an IAT study using event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 30(4), 1449–1457. (PMID: 16418007)
Maclean, A. M., Walker, L. J., & Matsuba, M. K. (2004). Transcendence and the moral self: Identity integration, religion, and moral life. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 43(3), 429–437.
Macmillan, N. A. (2002). Signal detection theory. Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley.
Magee, M. W., & Hardin, C. D. (2010). In defense of religion: Shared reality moderates the unconscious threat of evolution. Social Cognition, 28(3), 379–400.
Malka, A., Lelkes, Y., Srivastava, S., Cohen, A. B., & Miller, D. T. (2012). The association of religiosity and political conservatism: The role of political engagement. Political Psychology, 33(2), 275–299.
McCullough, M. E., & Willoughby, B. L. (2009). Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 69. (PMID: 19210054)
McKay, R., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Religion and morality. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 447. (PMID: 25528346)
Mikels, J. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Larkin, G. R., Lindberg, C. M., Maglio, S. J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005). Emotional category data on images from the International Affective Picture System. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 626–630. (PMID: 166292941808555)
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., & Eslinger, P. J. (2003). Morals and the human brain: a working model. NeuroReport, 14(3), 299–305. (PMID: 12634472)
Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Holmberg, E. B., & Hooley, J. M. (2008). The emotion reactivity scale: development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39(2), 107–116. (PMID: 18502244)
Noldus. (2018, February 23). Reference manual. Retrieved from http://www.noldus.com/human-behaviorresearch/products/facereader .
Pastore, R. E., Crawley, E. J., Berens, M. S., & Skelly, M. A. (2003). “Nonparametric” A’and other modern misconceptions about signal detection theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 556–569.
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13. (PMID: 172546362018741)
Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 188–196. (PMID: 15831401)
Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 773. (PMID: 209598603025529)
Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005a). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage, 28(1), 249–255. (PMID: 15993624)
Pessoa, L., Japee, S., Sturman, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2005b). Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16(3), 366–375. (PMID: 15930371)
Peterson, M., & Ruse, M. (2016). Science, evolution, and religion: A debate about atheism and theism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pichon, I., Boccato, G., & Saroglou, V. (2007). Nonconscious influences of religion on prosociality: A priming study. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(5), 1032–1045.
Plante, T. G., Vallaeys, C. L., Sherman, A. C., & Wallston, K. A. (2002). The development of a brief version of the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 50(5), 359–368.
Ponseti, J., & Bosinski, H. A. (2010). Subliminal sexual stimuli facilitate genital response in women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(5), 1073–1079. (PMID: 20041283)
Preston, J. L., & Ritter, R. S. (2012). Cleanliness and godliness: Mutual association between two kinds of personal purity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1365–1368.
Preston, J. L., Ritter, R. S., & Hernandez, J. I. (2010). Principles of religious prosociality: A review and reformulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(8), 574–590.
Qidwai, U., & Chen, C. H. (2009). Digital image processing: an algorithmic approach with MATLAB. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
Qing Leong, M., Yu, Z., Tsikandilakis, M. & M. W Tong., E. (2021) Religiosity and Physiological Responses to Immortality: An Exploration of Potentially Unconscious Moral Processes. Biological Psychology.
Ritter, R. S., & Preston, J. L. (2011). Gross gods and icky atheism: Disgust responses to rejected religious beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 1225–1230.
Ritter, R. S., Preston, J. L., Salomon, E., & Relihan-Johnson, D. (2016). Imagine no religion: Heretical disgust, anger and the symbolic purity of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 778–796. (PMID: 25899719)
Robin, X., Turck, N., Hainard, A., Tiberti, N., Lisacek, F., Sanchez, J. C., & Müller, M. (2011). pROC: an open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics, 12(1), 77. (PMID: 214142083068975)
Rossman, A. J., Short, T. H., & Parks, M. T. (1998). Bayes estimators for the continuous uniform distribution. Journal of Statistics Education, 6(3).
Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 329–349. (PMID: 12415074)
Samuel, L. R. (2016). Distinctly un-American: subliminal advertising and the Cold War. Journal of Historical Research in Marketing, 8(1), 99–119.
Saroglou, V., & Cohen, A. B. (2011). Psychology of culture and religion: Introduction to the JCCP special issue. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(8), 1309–1319.
Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: how the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist, 54(2), 93.
Shariff, A. F. (2015). Does religion increase moral behavior? Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 108–113.
Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), 27–48. (PMID: 25673322)
Sharrock, A. (2019). Intratextuality. Oxford: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Classics.
Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behaviour research methods, instruments, & computers, 31(1), 137–149.
Sun, R. (2012). Grounding social sciences in cognitive sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Swets, J. A. (2014). Signal detection theory and ROC analysis in psychology and diagnostics: Collected papers. Hove: Psychology Press.
Tendeiro, J. N., & Kiers, H. A. (2019). A review of issues about null hypothesis Bayesian testing. Psychological Methods, 24(6), 774. (PMID: 31094544)
Tsikandilakis, M., Chapman, P., & Peirce, J. (2018). Target meta-awareness is a necessary condition for physiological responses to masked emotional faces: Evidence from combined skin conductance and heart rate assessment. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 75–89. (PMID: 29239755)
Tsikandilakis, M., Bali, P., Derrfuss, J., & Chapman, P. (2019a). The unconscious mind: From classical theoretical controversy to controversial contemporary research and a practical illustration of the “error of our ways.” Consciousness and Cognition, 74, 102771. (PMID: 31299420)
Tsikandilakis, M., Kausel, L., Boncompte, G., Yu, Z., Oxner, M., Lanfranco, R., & Tong, E. M. W. (2019b). “There is no face like home”: ratings for cultural familiarity to own and other facial dialects of emotion with and without conscious awareness in a British sample. Perception, 48(10), 918–947. (PMID: 31451029)
Tsikandilakis, M., Bali, P., & Chapman, P. (2019c). Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: the appraisal of facial attractiveness and its relation to conscious awareness. Perception, 48(1), 72–92. (PMID: 30567468)
Tsikandilakis, M., Bali, P., Derrfuss, J., & Chapman, P. (2020a). “I can see you; I can feel it; and vice-versa”: consciousness and its relation to emotional physiology. Cognition and Emotion, 1–13.
Tsikandilakis, M., Bali, P., Derrfuss, J., & Chapman, P. (2020b). Anger and hostility: are they different? An analytical exploration of facial-expressive differences, and physiological and facial-emotional responses. Cognition and Emotion, 1–15.
Tsikandilakis, M., Bali, P., Yu, Z., Madan, C., Derrfuss, J., Chapman, P., & Groeger, J. (2021a): Subjective conscious and unconscious perception: theoretical and methodological conundrums, an experimental illustration, and its outcomes. Perception. (In Press).
Tsikandilakis, M., & Chapman, P. (2018). Skin-conductance responses to masked emotional faces are modulated by hit rate but not signal detection theory adjustments for subjective differences in the detection threshold. Perception, 47(4), 432–450. (PMID: 29466909)
Tsikandilakis , M., Yu, Z., Kausel, L., Boncompte, G., Lanfranco, R., Oxner, M., Bali, P., Urale, P., Leong, M. Q., Paterakis, G., Caci, S., Tong, E. M. W., Millbank, A., Mevel, A., Carmel, D., Madan, C., Derrfuss, J., Chapman, P. (2021b). There is no (where a) face like home”: recognition and appraisal responses to masked facial dialects of emotion in four different world cultures. Cognition. (In Press).
Tybur, J. M., Merriman, L. A., Hooper, A. E. C., McDonald, M. M., & Navarrete, C. D. (2010). Extending the behavioral immune system to political psychology: Are political conservatism and disgust sensitivity really related? Evolutionary Psychology, 8(4), 14747.
van der Ploeg, M. M., Brosschot, J. F., Versluis, A., & Verkuil, B. (2017). Peripheral physiological responses to subliminally presented negative affective stimuli: A systematic review. Biological Psychology, 129, 131–153. (PMID: 28843622)
van Slyke, J. A. (2016). The cognitive science of religion. London: Routledge.
Vanderhasselt, M. A., Kühn, S., & De Raedt, R. (2012). ‘Put on your poker face’: neural systems supporting the anticipation for expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 903–910. (PMID: 229566753831557)
Visala, A. (2016). Naturalism, theism and the cognitive study of religion: Religion explained? London: Routledge.
Warren, C. (2009). Subliminal stimuli, perception, and influence: A review of important studies and conclusions. American Journal of Media Psychology, 2(3/4), 189–210.
Westh, P. (2009). Anthropomorphism in god concepts: The role of narrative. Origins of Religion, Cognition and Culture, 1–21.
Wetherill, R. R., Childress, A. R., Jagannathan, K., Bender, J., Young, K. A., Suh, J. J., & Franklin, T. R. (2014). Neural responses to subliminally presented cannabis and other emotionally evocative cues in cannabis-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 231(7), 1397–1407.
Whitehead, J. D., & Whitehead, E. E. (2001). From consciousness to conscience reflections on the axial age. Fudan Journal (Social Sciences Edition), 4, 22.
Williams, L. M., Barton, M. J., Kemp, A. H., Liddell, B. J., Peduto, A., Gordon, E., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Distinct amygdala–autonomic arousal profiles in response to fear signals in healthy males and females. Neuroimage, 28(3), 618–626. (PMID: 16081303)
Zhang, J., & Mueller, S. T. (2005). A note on ROC analysis and non-parametric estimate of sensitivity. Psychometrika, 70(1), 203–212.
Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K. I., Cole, B., Rye, M. S., Butfer, E. M., Belavich, T. G., & Kadar, J. L. (2015). Religion and spirituality: Unfuzzying the fuzzy. Sociology of Religion. 29–34.
تواريخ الأحداث: Date Created: 20210123 Date Completed: 20220209 Latest Revision: 20220209
رمز التحديث: 20231215
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-020-01461-7
PMID: 33484351
قاعدة البيانات: MEDLINE
الوصف
تدمد:1430-2772
DOI:10.1007/s00426-020-01461-7