دورية أكاديمية

Behavioral and electroencephalographic evidence for reduced attentional control and performance monitoring in boredom.

التفاصيل البيبلوغرافية
العنوان: Behavioral and electroencephalographic evidence for reduced attentional control and performance monitoring in boredom.
المؤلفون: Yakobi O; Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada., Boylan J; Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada., Danckert J; Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada.
المصدر: Psychophysiology [Psychophysiology] 2021 Jun; Vol. 58 (6), pp. e13816. Date of Electronic Publication: 2021 Mar 25.
نوع المنشور: Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
اللغة: English
بيانات الدورية: Publisher: Blackwell Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 0142657 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1540-5958 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00485772 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Psychophysiology Subsets: MEDLINE
أسماء مطبوعة: Publication: Malden, MA : Blackwell
Original Publication: Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins.
مواضيع طبية MeSH: Boredom* , Electroencephalography* , Task Performance and Analysis*, Attention/*physiology , Event-Related Potentials, P300/*physiology, Adult ; Brain/physiology ; Female ; Humans ; Male ; Neuropsychological Tests ; Young Adult
مستخلص: Boredom, the unfulfilled desire to be engaged in a satisfying mental activity, is an aversive state characterized by poor self-regulation. There is ample evidence that both state and trait boredom are associated with failures of attention in both experimental and everyday settings. The neural correlates of boredom, however, remain underexplored. We recorded electroencephalographic signal from 83 participants during a resting state and while performing a go/no-go task. We found a negative correlation between trait boredom proneness and power in the alpha and theta bands during the resting state. Furthermore, higher levels of task-induced boredom were associated with reduced amplitudes for the P3 and error-related negativity event-related potentials. Increased commission error rates were also associated with higher levels of task-induced boredom. We conclude that state and trait boredom are associated with inadequate engagement of attentional resources.
(© 2021 Society for Psychophysiological Research.)
References: Arns, M., Conners, C. K., & Kraemer, H. C. (2013). A decade of EEG theta/beta ratio research in ADHD: A meta-analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17(5), 374-383. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712460087.
Babiloni, C., Stella, G., Buffo, P., Vecchio, F., Onorati, P., Muratori, C., Miano, S., Gheller, F., Antonaci, L., Albertini, G., & Rossini, P. M. (2012). Cortical sources of resting state EEG rhythms are abnormal in dyslexic children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(12), 2384-2391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2012.05.002.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.
Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 35-42. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1990.67.1.35.
Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 835-847. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008.
Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414-421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012.
Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., Klein, T. J., & Allen, J. J. B. (2010). Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning. Neuroimage, 49(4), 3198-3209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.080.
Corcoran, A. W., Alday, P. M., Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2018). Toward a reliable, automated method of individual alpha frequency (IAF) quantification. Psychophysiology, 55(7), e13064. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13064.
Danckert, J. (2019). Boredom: Managing the delicate balance between exploration and exploitation. In J. Ros Velasco (Ed.), Boredom is in your mind: A shared psychological-philosophical approach (1st ed., pp. 37-53). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26395-9_3.
Danckert, J., & Isacescu, J. (2017). The bored brain: Insular cortex and the default mode network. OSF. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/aqbcd.
Danckert, J., & Merrifield, C. (2018). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2507-2518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4617-5.
Danckert, J., Mugon, J., Struk, A. A., & Eastwood, J. (2018). Boredom: What is it good for? In H. C. Lench (Ed.), The function of emotions (pp. 93-119). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77619-4.
Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise!…Surprise? Psychophysiology, 18(5), 493-513. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01815.x.
Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. H. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(3), 357-374. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00058027.
Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Stadler, W., Pöllhuber, D., & Heine, C. (2002). EEG alpha power and intelligence. Intelligence, 30(3), 289-302. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00101-5.
Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482-495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044.
Elpidorou, A. (2020). Is boredom one or many? A functional solution to the problem of heterogeneity. Mind and Language, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12282.
Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error detection, and correction. Neuroimage, 17(4), 1820-1829. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1326.
Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, E. A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96(14), 8301-8306. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301.
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385-390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x.
Golubchik, P., Manor, I., Shoval, G., & Weizman, A. (2020). Levels of proneness to boredom in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on and off methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 30(3), 173-176. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2019.0151.
Hsu, C. F., Eastwood, J. D., Toplak, M. E., Liang, J. C., Hwang-Gu, S. L., & Chen, V. C. H. (2020). Trait and state boredom: Associations with attention failure in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 286, 112861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112861.
Hunter, A., & Eastwood, J. D. (2018). Does state boredom cause failures of attention? Examining the relations between trait boredom, state boredom, and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2483-2492. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4749-7.
Katahira, K., Yamazaki, Y., Yamaoka, C., Ozaki, H., Nakagawa, S., & Nagata, N. (2018). EEG correlates of the flow state: A combination of increased frontal theta and moderate frontocentral alpha rhythm in the mental arithmetic task. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00300.
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 63-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003.
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 38(3), 557-577. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201990559.
Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(06), 661-679. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x12003196.
Lee, F. K. S., & Zelman, D. C. (2019). Boredom proneness as a predictor of depression, anxiety and stress: The moderating effects of dispositional mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 68-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.001.
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.1118/1.4736938.
Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y., & Danckert, J. (2012). Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research, 221(1), 59-67. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3147-z.
Mittner, M., Hawkins, G. E., Boekel, W., & Forstmann, B. U. (2016). A neural model of mind wandering. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 570-578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.004.
Mo, J., Liu, Y., Huang, H., & Ding, M. (2013). Coupling between visual alpha oscillations and default mode activity. Neuroimage, 68, 112-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.058.
Núñez Castellar, E. P., Antons, J. N., Marinazzo, D., & Van Looy, J. (2019). Mapping attention during gameplay: Assessment of behavioral and ERP markers in an auditory oddball task. Psychophysiology, 56(7), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13347.
Orcutt, J. D. (1984). Contrasting effects of two kinds of boredom on alcohol use. Journal of Drug Issues, 14(1), 161-173. https://doi.org/10.1177/002204268401400112.
Polich, J. (2003). Theoretical overview of P3a and P3b. In J. Polich (Ed.), Detection of change (1st ed., pp. 83-98. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0294-4_5.
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128-2148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019.
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676-682. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676.
Redick, T. S., Calvo, A., Gay, C. E., & Engle, R. W. (2011). Working memory capacity and go/no-go task performance: Selective effects of updating, maintenance, and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37(2), 308-324. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022216.
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747-758. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8.
Ryman, S. G., Cavanagh, J. F., Wertz, C. J., Shaff, N. A., Dodd, A. B., Stevens, B., Ling, J., Yeo, R. A., Hanlon, F. M., Bustillo, J., Stromberg, S. F., Lin, D. S., Abrams, S., & Mayer, A. R. (2018). Impaired midline theta power and connectivity during proactive cognitive control in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 84(9), 675-683. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.021.
Scheffers, M. K., & Coles, M. G. (2000). Performance monitoring in a confusing world: Error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 141-151. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.1.141.
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217-240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007.
Singh, A., Richardson, S. P., Narayanan, N., & Cavanagh, J. F. (2018). Mid-frontal theta activity is diminished during cognitive control in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 117, 113-122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.020.
Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346-359. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115609996.
Tabatabaie, A. F., Azadehfar, M. R., Mirian, N., Noroozian, M., Yoonessi, A., Saebipour, M. R., & Yoonessi, A. (2014). Neural correlates of boredom in music perception. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 5(4), 259-266.
van Meel, C. S., Heslenfeld, D. J., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2007). Adaptive control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The role of error processing. Psychiatry Research, 151(3), 211-220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.011.
Vodanovich, S. J. (2005). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychological Reports, 69(8), 1139. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.69.8.1139-1146.
Westgate, E. C., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Boring thoughts and bored minds: The MAC model of boredom and cognitive engagement. Psychological Review, 125(5), 689-713. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000097.
Yakobi, O. (2018). Determinants of association and dissociation between subjective and objective measures of workload. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 62(1), 222-226. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541931218621052.
فهرسة مساهمة: Keywords: EEG; P3; boredom; error-related negativity; frequency analysis; sustained attention
تواريخ الأحداث: Date Created: 20210326 Date Completed: 20220202 Latest Revision: 20220202
رمز التحديث: 20240513
DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13816
PMID: 33768592
قاعدة البيانات: MEDLINE
الوصف
تدمد:1540-5958
DOI:10.1111/psyp.13816