دورية أكاديمية

The odour of an unfamiliar stressed or relaxed person affects dogs' responses to a cognitive bias test.

التفاصيل البيبلوغرافية
العنوان: The odour of an unfamiliar stressed or relaxed person affects dogs' responses to a cognitive bias test.
المؤلفون: Parr-Cortes Z; Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, UK. zc0342@my.bristol.ac.uk.; School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF10 3AX, UK. zc0342@my.bristol.ac.uk., Müller CT; School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF10 3AX, UK., Talas L; Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, UK., Mendl M; Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, UK., Guest C; Medical Detection Dogs, Milton Keynes, MK17 0NP, UK., Rooney NJ; Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, UK.; Medical Detection Dogs, Milton Keynes, MK17 0NP, UK.
المصدر: Scientific reports [Sci Rep] 2024 Jul 22; Vol. 14 (1), pp. 15843. Date of Electronic Publication: 2024 Jul 22.
نوع المنشور: Journal Article
اللغة: English
بيانات الدورية: Publisher: Nature Publishing Group Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 101563288 Publication Model: Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 2045-2322 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 20452322 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Sci Rep Subsets: MEDLINE
أسماء مطبوعة: Original Publication: London : Nature Publishing Group, copyright 2011-
مواضيع طبية MeSH: Odorants* , Cognition*/physiology , Stress, Psychological* , Behavior, Animal*/physiology, Animals ; Dogs ; Humans ; Male ; Female ; Smell/physiology ; Cues ; Learning/physiology
مستخلص: Dogs can discriminate stressed from non-stressed human odour samples, but the effect on their cognition is unstudied. Using a cognitive bias task, we tested how human odours affect dogs' likelihood of approaching a food bowl placed at three ambiguous locations ("near-positive", "middle" and "near-negative") between trained "positive" (rewarded) and "negative" (unrewarded) locations. Using odour samples collected from three unfamiliar volunteers during stressful and relaxing activities, we tested eighteen dogs under three conditions: no odour, stress odour and relaxed odour, with the order of test odours counterbalanced across dogs. When exposed to stress odour during session three, dogs were significantly less likely to approach a bowl placed at one of the three ambiguous locations (near-negative) compared to no odour, indicating possible risk-reduction behaviours in response to the smell of human stress. Dogs' learning of trained positive and negative locations improved with repeated testing and was significant between sessions two and three only when exposed to stress odour during session three, suggesting odour influenced learning. This is the first study to show that without visual or auditory cues, olfactory cues of human stress may affect dogs' cognition and learning, which, if true, could have important consequences for dog welfare and working performance.
(© 2024. The Author(s).)
References: Learn Mem. 2019 Mar 21;26(4):121-127. (PMID: 30898974)
Appetite. 2013 Aug;67:74-80. (PMID: 23583314)
PLoS One. 2016 Nov 9;11(11):e0165840. (PMID: 27829041)
Neurobiol Stress. 2016 Nov 12;6:113-126. (PMID: 28229114)
J Comp Psychol. 2002 Mar;116(1):27-34. (PMID: 11926681)
Behav Brain Res. 2016 May 1;304:34-41. (PMID: 26876141)
Physiol Behav. 1998 Feb 15;63(4):489-95. (PMID: 9523888)
Chem Senses. 2011 Jan;36(1):19-27. (PMID: 20929974)
J Comp Psychol. 2017 Nov;131(4):317-325. (PMID: 28517942)
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2024 Feb;157:105527. (PMID: 38160722)
BMC Vet Res. 2015 Mar 28;11:80. (PMID: 25889323)
Neural Plast. 2007;2007:78970. (PMID: 18060012)
J Breath Res. 2016 Nov 21;10(4):046012. (PMID: 27869110)
PLoS One. 2019 Jan 30;14(1):e0211363. (PMID: 30699168)
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1998 Apr;136(3):272-83. (PMID: 9566813)
Sci Rep. 2020 Oct 20;10(1):17837. (PMID: 33082433)
Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001 Apr 26;72(2):145-171. (PMID: 11278033)
Anim Cogn. 2020 Jul;23(4):721-729. (PMID: 32270350)
Chem Senses. 2017 Oct 1;42(8):663-673. (PMID: 28981822)
Anim Cogn. 2023 Mar;26(2):477-489. (PMID: 36094748)
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000 Jan;57(1):14-20. (PMID: 10632228)
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005 May;29(3):469-91. (PMID: 15820551)
Behav Pharmacol. 2012 Sep;23(5-6):442-60. (PMID: 22854304)
Animals (Basel). 2019 Oct 31;9(11):. (PMID: 31683710)
Front Behav Neurosci. 2019 Mar 20;13:54. (PMID: 30949035)
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014 Jan;38:94-124. (PMID: 24239854)
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 Aug;36(7):1740-9. (PMID: 21771612)
Front Syst Neurosci. 2013 Oct 10;7:66. (PMID: 24124415)
Curr Biol. 2012 Feb 7;22(3):209-12. (PMID: 22226744)
Behav Brain Res. 2000 Dec 20;117(1-2):137-46. (PMID: 11099767)
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020 May;112:144-163. (PMID: 31991192)
Nature. 2004 Jan 22;427(6972):312. (PMID: 14737158)
Anim Behav. 2015 Aug;106:27-35. (PMID: 26257403)
Anim Cogn. 2016 Nov;19(6):1231-1235. (PMID: 27338818)
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2013 May;4(3):245-261. (PMID: 26304203)
Physiol Behav. 2001 Jan;72(1-2):45-50. (PMID: 11239980)
Sci Rep. 2018 Apr 27;8(1):6660. (PMID: 29703989)
Curr Biol. 2010 Oct 12;20(19):R839-40. (PMID: 20937467)
J Breath Res. 2019 Apr 16;13(3):036003. (PMID: 30790788)
Biol Psychol. 2021 May;162:108114. (PMID: 33979670)
Behav Brain Res. 1999 Dec;106(1-2):133-42. (PMID: 10595429)
Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017 Apr;140:134-144. (PMID: 28267591)
Anim Cogn. 2021 Jan;24(1):107-119. (PMID: 32897444)
Genome Res. 2016 Feb;26(2):163-73. (PMID: 26680994)
J Vis Exp. 2011 Oct 19;(56):. (PMID: 22042290)
Curr Biol. 2015 Jun 1;25(11):1515-9. (PMID: 26004765)
J Exp Psychol Gen. 2019 Jan;148(1):13-29. (PMID: 30221962)
Neurosci Lett. 2006 Feb 13;394(2):127-30. (PMID: 16257486)
Top Cogn Sci. 2023 May 18;:. (PMID: 37203276)
Vet J. 2003 May;165(3):296-301. (PMID: 12672376)
PLoS One. 2022 Sep 28;17(9):e0274143. (PMID: 36170254)
Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019 Nov 27;16(23):. (PMID: 31783531)
Chem Senses. 2016 Jan;41(1):35-43. (PMID: 26453051)
Science. 2004 Jun 11;304(5677):1682-3. (PMID: 15192233)
Chem Senses. 2011 Jul;36(6):555-64. (PMID: 21444931)
Behav Processes. 2010 Mar;83(3):349-52. (PMID: 20117188)
Biol Lett. 2016 Jan;12(1):20150883. (PMID: 26763220)
J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014 Apr;143(2):825-34. (PMID: 23855495)
Behav Processes. 2015 Jan;110:37-46. (PMID: 24607363)
J Comp Psychol. 2003 Sep;117(3):257-63. (PMID: 14498801)
Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1-2):76-81. (PMID: 8255414)
Sci Rep. 2021 Sep 24;11(1):19023. (PMID: 34561478)
J Neurosci. 2022 Aug 17;42(33):6392-6407. (PMID: 35817576)
Anim Cogn. 2023 Jul;26(4):1241-1250. (PMID: 37010698)
Proc Biol Sci. 2011 Oct 22;278(1721):3129-34. (PMID: 21389025)
Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2014;113(1):210-20. (PMID: 24054542)
Animals (Basel). 2022 Feb 01;12(3):. (PMID: 35158677)
Animals (Basel). 2018 Jul 31;8(8):. (PMID: 30065156)
Anim Cogn. 2018 Mar;21(2):253-265. (PMID: 29442250)
Anim Cogn. 2018 Jan;21(1):67-78. (PMID: 28988316)
Psychol Sci. 2009 Feb;20(2):177-83. (PMID: 19170944)
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014 Dec 30;111(52):18751-6. (PMID: 25512532)
J Dairy Sci. 2024 Feb;107(2):1085-1101. (PMID: 37709022)
معلومات مُعتمدة: BB/M009122/1 United Kingdom BB_ Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council
فهرسة مساهمة: Keywords: Animal cognition; Dogs; Emotions; Human chemosignals; Stress
تواريخ الأحداث: Date Created: 20240722 Date Completed: 20240722 Latest Revision: 20240730
رمز التحديث: 20240730
مُعرف محوري في PubMed: PMC11263577
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-66147-1
PMID: 39039125
قاعدة البيانات: MEDLINE
الوصف
تدمد:2045-2322
DOI:10.1038/s41598-024-66147-1